Saturday, 29 March 2014
Sunny schooling
Another good session with Jane, still painful at end for me I was slightly concerned re his breathing as at some times in trot there was a 'grunt' sound no signs of respiratory distress and seemed happy enough to go forwards
Labels:
Jane Condon
Friday, 28 March 2014
Lesson, lameness and pyjamas
So lesson at 08:00 and it was a bit brr, pelvis had been playing up night before but other than feeling a bit queasy wasn't in pain.
Boy is more forwards, but had tendency to lean am wondering if a Neue Schule Verbinded Bit would help? Generally I was always taught it isn't the bit it is the rider. Had to stop early as started to get abdominal pain again so have been madly doing my stretch exercises to try and ease it off but I felt quite lame once dismounted.
Have bought the boys
these in the Snuggy Hoods sale as thought would be excellent for winter as can stay on when changing from turnout to stable rugs.
Boy is more forwards, but had tendency to lean am wondering if a Neue Schule Verbinded Bit would help? Generally I was always taught it isn't the bit it is the rider. Had to stop early as started to get abdominal pain again so have been madly doing my stretch exercises to try and ease it off but I felt quite lame once dismounted.
Have bought the boys
these in the Snuggy Hoods sale as thought would be excellent for winter as can stay on when changing from turnout to stable rugs.
Labels:
Jane Condon,
Random ramblings
Wednesday, 26 March 2014
Pony does poles
Did poles in hand with the pony today as per physio instructions.
Clever pony picked his feet up!
Clever pony picked his feet up!
Labels:
Nicky Desailly
Monday, 24 March 2014
Osteopath - third visit
Very pleased with my side bend range of movement, but pelvis still dropped left.
Went back to level much more easily but he is concerned the 'fix' is not taking so have been given a 9mm heel list to wear in my left shoe to align my pelvis.
This will help my muscles to start to return to normal function.
Suspect have been out since the severe abdominal pain back in 2008 as he think it unlikely I'd have straightened up without help and have no idea how straight it was in the time period all I know was that come the sciatica in April 2013 it was very jammed up and crooked.
So heel lift for two weeks and review then four more weeks with a review and maybe that will be it job done, I can but hope.
Went back to level much more easily but he is concerned the 'fix' is not taking so have been given a 9mm heel list to wear in my left shoe to align my pelvis.
This will help my muscles to start to return to normal function.
Suspect have been out since the severe abdominal pain back in 2008 as he think it unlikely I'd have straightened up without help and have no idea how straight it was in the time period all I know was that come the sciatica in April 2013 it was very jammed up and crooked.
So heel lift for two weeks and review then four more weeks with a review and maybe that will be it job done, I can but hope.
Labels:
Osteopath
Sunday, 23 March 2014
I rode :-) and seven years
Finally after just over three weeks I am back on board, have been doing my exercises like something possessed and maybe it has indeed paid off?
There is still some crookedness but it wasn't impacting Chorrie
Today is the seventh anniversary of my purchasing the boy, in some ways it seems longer and in others only yesterday
It has been a roller coaster seven years but every day has been enhanced for his being in it
Labels:
Random ramblings
Wednesday, 19 March 2014
A bear of very little brain
Seriously ?
If I had a fully functioning brain I'd be dangerous, have been in a woe as have been thinking Chorrie's canter hasn't cone back as it was pre injury, well that may be true but looking at video of some good horses his hind leg cones as far under as theirs
Hey ho
In other news field has been rolled which always feels like Winter's end, nights have drawn out, it is light well before 06:00
And I still can't bloody ride!
If I had a fully functioning brain I'd be dangerous, have been in a woe as have been thinking Chorrie's canter hasn't cone back as it was pre injury, well that may be true but looking at video of some good horses his hind leg cones as far under as theirs
Hey ho
In other news field has been rolled which always feels like Winter's end, nights have drawn out, it is light well before 06:00
And I still can't bloody ride!
Labels:
Chorister - Check ligament
Tuesday, 18 March 2014
My pony my mirror
So physio out to Chorrie pony this morning.
Left side is not as bad as last time (two weeks ago) but there is still some 'stuff' going on.
Unlike me his pelvis is straight, but like me he is tight left side (chicken/egg)
Need to do raised poles with and lead him over so left side is nearest to the raised side.
Left side is not as bad as last time (two weeks ago) but there is still some 'stuff' going on.
Unlike me his pelvis is straight, but like me he is tight left side (chicken/egg)
Need to do raised poles with and lead him over so left side is nearest to the raised side.
Labels:
Nicky Desailly
Monday, 17 March 2014
The Wrong Continuum - Catherine Bell
http://www.network54.com/Forum/235380/thread/1395008197/last-1395008197/The+Wrong+Continuum+-+Catherine+Bell
The Wrong Continuum
It's probably fair to say that, when you start to study equine behaviour, one of the first things you learn is learning theory and, in particular, operant conditioning. To some people it appears delightfully straightforward; to others it is an alien science which seems far removed from our equine friends. Either way, it becomes a fairly crucial tool in our interactions with horses. Operant conditioning is employed every time we apply or release some form of pressure and, since the vast majority of instructions given to a horse by a human involve pressure, it is something we do all the time. Whether or not we are aware of it. Whether or not we like it.
Operant conditioning can be broken down into four "contingencies". We have reinforcement and punishment and each of these can be positive or negative. That's it. If a behaviour becomes more likely to reoccur in the future then we say it has been reinforced - positively if we have added something (e.g. given a reward) or negatively if we have taken something away (e.g. release of pressure). If a behaviour becomes less likely to reoccur in the future then we say it has been punished - positively if we have added something (e.g. a smack) or negatively if we have taken something away (e.g. stopped giving a scratch). Of course, all of these "somethings", otherwise known as "stimuli", need to be properly salient, or relevant, to the horse.
Unofficially, and somewhat emotionally, we all place these contingencies on a continuum of what we perceive to be more or less ethical. It all starts to get a bit confused. At one extreme we feel that hitting horses is pretty bad but "using a little tap of the whip to back up the leg" is perhaps acceptable. Negative punishment doesn't seem quite as bad as positive punishment. Negative reinforcement is normally considered the most practical compromise; we apply mild pressure (or sometimes quite considerable pressure) and the horse works to remove that pressure. And then we have positive reinforcement, seen by some as the "holy grail" of ethical training and by others as too impractical to implement sensibly.
So we have our continuum of bad to good. We feel good about our training when we can be up in the reinforcement end of the continuum and we feel bad about our training when we are down at the punishment end. And we have long arguments on internet forums when we think we are using milder punishment than other people who use much more aversive training. Are we being more ethical if we use positive reinforcement and is it always actually positive? Or could the need for strict stimulus control border on the aversive? What about when we combine positive reinforcement with negative reinforcement? Does that get us the "best of both worlds" - the ethical combined with the practical? These topics have all been the subject of my previous articles, all listed onhttp://www.equinemindandbody.co.uk/articles.html
But are we missing something? Did we ever stop to consider whether there is any more to behaviourism than just this combination of scientific contingency and emotional hierarchy? Is the science of behaviour complete? Or has the research continued? Unsurprisingly, the research has indeed continued, with far-reaching consequences - and the results have been somewhat surprising to anyone who has been raised on a diet of Skinner.
For a start, monkeys were found to perform tasks involving a mechanical puzzle for no reinforcement. The monkeys were clearly interested in the task for its own sake, an observation contrary to the stimulus-response pairing predicted by behaviourism, and the inclusion of food rewards disrupted their ability to solve the puzzle (Harlow 1953). Experiments into the motivation of humans found that giving monetary rewards actually reduces the likelihood of repeating the task in the absence of rewards (Deci 1995). In fact, in a wide array of studies of human behaviour from the fields of education, parenting and business, rewards actually turn out to be punishing, to rupture our relationships, to ignore the reasons behind behaviours, to discourage risk taking and to reduce interest in the task (Kohn 1993). Kohn also points out that most of the experiments leading to the results of behaviourism made use of semi-starved rats in under-enriched cages, or institutionalised adults in care or children who were already dependent on others to provide for them. If you are already dependent on reward systems to motivate you then you do indeed tend to continue to need rewards for your ongoing "motivation".
In fact, to quote career analyst Dan Pink in his entertaining and enlightening 2009 TED talk: "This has been replicated over and over and over again, for nearly 40 years. These contingent motivators - if you do this, then you get that - work in some circumstances. But for a lot of tasks, they actually either don't work or, often, they do harm. This is one of the most robust findings in social science, and also one of the most ignored."
In his book "Punished By Rewards" (I highly recommend this book to anyone who likes a detailed book with lots of references. Alternatively see the slightly lighter Unconditional Parenting instead. Preferably both.), Alfie Kohn makes a similar point - it's not that rewards categorically don't work. They do work, as all those of us who like to use clicker training (myself included) will agree. But perhaps it is a little sobering to look at the conditions under which rewards work best:
- they tend to benefit the person who decides what gets rewarded, rather than the recipient
- they tend to work only for as long as the rewards keep coming (hence many horses' resistance to the notion of "fading" the clicker/reward)
- they tend to work best for very mindless tasks with little or no intrinsic interest
I'm sorry to say that these conditions describe an awful lot of clicker training sessions I have witnessed. And sure, we can work to mitigate the negative effects of the training by keeping rewards low-key and unexpected (i.e. avoiding bribery and fixation on the rewards). But ultimately when we try to use reward-based training we have no guarantee that we are truly providing a positive experience for the animal. Even if we are providing tasty and desirable treats in the short-term, what is an intensive conditioning process doing to a horse's motivation, and to the relationship we have with our horses in the longer-term?
But can't we do better? What if we want to benefit our horses, maintain behaviours in the absence of rewards and not restrict ourselves to mindless tasks? And still not resort to aversive training? Is there anything else? Motivation expert, Ed Deci, states in his book "Why We Do What We Do" that intrinsic motivation is associated with a richer experience, better conceptual understanding, greater creativity and improved problem-solving, compared with extrinsic rewards. This sounds more like it - can we tap into that instead?
Happily, yes we can do better, there is something else and we can tap into intrinsic motivation instead. Deci highlights autonomy as one of our key basic needs. We need to feel that we have control over our lives. When we are being "trained", be it via positive or negative approaches, we feel controlled and lacking in motivation. We are no longer able to make choices. But if we can retain our autonomy we avoid those pitfalls. Is there any reason to assume that horses don't feel similarly?
Horses are typically domesticated and even those who are feral often have their lives encroached on by humans. We keep horses in stables, in small paddocks, alone or with companions they would not necessarily choose and ride them to suit our own wishes. How can we even begin to pretend they have their autonomy? But I am reminded of an interview with a survivor of the Russian Gulags who, when asked how frightening it must have been to be imprisoned, said that in the camps you were more free than when you were outside. Free to think and speak your own thoughts, rather than too frightened to express them as he had been previously. Autonomy is not restricted to our geographical location but is much more linked to how we feel about our situation. It gives us the freedom to be who we really are. We don't even need to experience autonomy all the time - in the wonderful book "Dibs: In Search of Self", therapist Virginia Axline describes the phenomenal and life-changing personal growth of a six year old boy in just an hour-long session of autonomous play therapy each week, despite his somewhat aversive home life.
What would autonomy in horse training look like? Is it an unavoidable oxymoron or can we do something that approaches it for the benefit of our horses? In order to support the autonomy of humans in a therapeutic manner, Deci advises us to take the clients perspective, acknowledge the others feelings, provide relevant information, give rationale for any suggestions or requests that we make, offer choice and control to the client and minimise the use of controlling language. With our horses we can translate this into doing our best to see all of our requests from the point of view of the horse and acknowledge that the horse may have a very different feeling about what, to us, might appear an easy task. We can't use language to provide the relevant information or rationale behind any requests but we can take time for the horse to really understand what we would like and use shaping to make any task non-aversive and as easy to achieve as possible. We can reduce the use of controlling training by allowing the horse to make choices and decisions and we can pay attention to the answers we are given. We can make use of occasional free-shaping sessions to give the horse the opportunity to choose to perform behaviours to elicit rewards, without losing sight of the controlling nature of reward-based training. The goal here is for the horse to be making decisions, rather than merely performing behaviours of our choosing. And couple all of this with meeting his ethological needs as best we can. And when we do all of this, it is remarkable how much easier it is to make requests of them.
So what is the right continuum? I would put all forms of punishment, reinforcement, control and extrinsic motivation at the "bad end" and choice, empowerment, autonomy and intrinsic motivation at the "good" end. And that is the case for myself, my children and my animals. We can't avoid the "bad end" entirely but by maximising the opportunities for autonomy, we can work towards returning some of that long-lost fundamental need to our animals.
References
Virginia Axline, 1964, "Dibs: In Search of Self"
Ed Deci, 1995, "Why We Do What We Do"
Harry Harlow, 1953, "Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses." In "Current theory and research on motivation (p 24-49), Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press
Alfie Kohn, 1993, "Punished By Rewards"
The Wrong Continuum
It's probably fair to say that, when you start to study equine behaviour, one of the first things you learn is learning theory and, in particular, operant conditioning. To some people it appears delightfully straightforward; to others it is an alien science which seems far removed from our equine friends. Either way, it becomes a fairly crucial tool in our interactions with horses. Operant conditioning is employed every time we apply or release some form of pressure and, since the vast majority of instructions given to a horse by a human involve pressure, it is something we do all the time. Whether or not we are aware of it. Whether or not we like it.
Operant conditioning can be broken down into four "contingencies". We have reinforcement and punishment and each of these can be positive or negative. That's it. If a behaviour becomes more likely to reoccur in the future then we say it has been reinforced - positively if we have added something (e.g. given a reward) or negatively if we have taken something away (e.g. release of pressure). If a behaviour becomes less likely to reoccur in the future then we say it has been punished - positively if we have added something (e.g. a smack) or negatively if we have taken something away (e.g. stopped giving a scratch). Of course, all of these "somethings", otherwise known as "stimuli", need to be properly salient, or relevant, to the horse.
Unofficially, and somewhat emotionally, we all place these contingencies on a continuum of what we perceive to be more or less ethical. It all starts to get a bit confused. At one extreme we feel that hitting horses is pretty bad but "using a little tap of the whip to back up the leg" is perhaps acceptable. Negative punishment doesn't seem quite as bad as positive punishment. Negative reinforcement is normally considered the most practical compromise; we apply mild pressure (or sometimes quite considerable pressure) and the horse works to remove that pressure. And then we have positive reinforcement, seen by some as the "holy grail" of ethical training and by others as too impractical to implement sensibly.
So we have our continuum of bad to good. We feel good about our training when we can be up in the reinforcement end of the continuum and we feel bad about our training when we are down at the punishment end. And we have long arguments on internet forums when we think we are using milder punishment than other people who use much more aversive training. Are we being more ethical if we use positive reinforcement and is it always actually positive? Or could the need for strict stimulus control border on the aversive? What about when we combine positive reinforcement with negative reinforcement? Does that get us the "best of both worlds" - the ethical combined with the practical? These topics have all been the subject of my previous articles, all listed onhttp://www.equinemindandbody.co.uk/articles.html
But are we missing something? Did we ever stop to consider whether there is any more to behaviourism than just this combination of scientific contingency and emotional hierarchy? Is the science of behaviour complete? Or has the research continued? Unsurprisingly, the research has indeed continued, with far-reaching consequences - and the results have been somewhat surprising to anyone who has been raised on a diet of Skinner.
For a start, monkeys were found to perform tasks involving a mechanical puzzle for no reinforcement. The monkeys were clearly interested in the task for its own sake, an observation contrary to the stimulus-response pairing predicted by behaviourism, and the inclusion of food rewards disrupted their ability to solve the puzzle (Harlow 1953). Experiments into the motivation of humans found that giving monetary rewards actually reduces the likelihood of repeating the task in the absence of rewards (Deci 1995). In fact, in a wide array of studies of human behaviour from the fields of education, parenting and business, rewards actually turn out to be punishing, to rupture our relationships, to ignore the reasons behind behaviours, to discourage risk taking and to reduce interest in the task (Kohn 1993). Kohn also points out that most of the experiments leading to the results of behaviourism made use of semi-starved rats in under-enriched cages, or institutionalised adults in care or children who were already dependent on others to provide for them. If you are already dependent on reward systems to motivate you then you do indeed tend to continue to need rewards for your ongoing "motivation".
In fact, to quote career analyst Dan Pink in his entertaining and enlightening 2009 TED talk: "This has been replicated over and over and over again, for nearly 40 years. These contingent motivators - if you do this, then you get that - work in some circumstances. But for a lot of tasks, they actually either don't work or, often, they do harm. This is one of the most robust findings in social science, and also one of the most ignored."
In his book "Punished By Rewards" (I highly recommend this book to anyone who likes a detailed book with lots of references. Alternatively see the slightly lighter Unconditional Parenting instead. Preferably both.), Alfie Kohn makes a similar point - it's not that rewards categorically don't work. They do work, as all those of us who like to use clicker training (myself included) will agree. But perhaps it is a little sobering to look at the conditions under which rewards work best:
- they tend to benefit the person who decides what gets rewarded, rather than the recipient
- they tend to work only for as long as the rewards keep coming (hence many horses' resistance to the notion of "fading" the clicker/reward)
- they tend to work best for very mindless tasks with little or no intrinsic interest
I'm sorry to say that these conditions describe an awful lot of clicker training sessions I have witnessed. And sure, we can work to mitigate the negative effects of the training by keeping rewards low-key and unexpected (i.e. avoiding bribery and fixation on the rewards). But ultimately when we try to use reward-based training we have no guarantee that we are truly providing a positive experience for the animal. Even if we are providing tasty and desirable treats in the short-term, what is an intensive conditioning process doing to a horse's motivation, and to the relationship we have with our horses in the longer-term?
But can't we do better? What if we want to benefit our horses, maintain behaviours in the absence of rewards and not restrict ourselves to mindless tasks? And still not resort to aversive training? Is there anything else? Motivation expert, Ed Deci, states in his book "Why We Do What We Do" that intrinsic motivation is associated with a richer experience, better conceptual understanding, greater creativity and improved problem-solving, compared with extrinsic rewards. This sounds more like it - can we tap into that instead?
Happily, yes we can do better, there is something else and we can tap into intrinsic motivation instead. Deci highlights autonomy as one of our key basic needs. We need to feel that we have control over our lives. When we are being "trained", be it via positive or negative approaches, we feel controlled and lacking in motivation. We are no longer able to make choices. But if we can retain our autonomy we avoid those pitfalls. Is there any reason to assume that horses don't feel similarly?
Horses are typically domesticated and even those who are feral often have their lives encroached on by humans. We keep horses in stables, in small paddocks, alone or with companions they would not necessarily choose and ride them to suit our own wishes. How can we even begin to pretend they have their autonomy? But I am reminded of an interview with a survivor of the Russian Gulags who, when asked how frightening it must have been to be imprisoned, said that in the camps you were more free than when you were outside. Free to think and speak your own thoughts, rather than too frightened to express them as he had been previously. Autonomy is not restricted to our geographical location but is much more linked to how we feel about our situation. It gives us the freedom to be who we really are. We don't even need to experience autonomy all the time - in the wonderful book "Dibs: In Search of Self", therapist Virginia Axline describes the phenomenal and life-changing personal growth of a six year old boy in just an hour-long session of autonomous play therapy each week, despite his somewhat aversive home life.
What would autonomy in horse training look like? Is it an unavoidable oxymoron or can we do something that approaches it for the benefit of our horses? In order to support the autonomy of humans in a therapeutic manner, Deci advises us to take the clients perspective, acknowledge the others feelings, provide relevant information, give rationale for any suggestions or requests that we make, offer choice and control to the client and minimise the use of controlling language. With our horses we can translate this into doing our best to see all of our requests from the point of view of the horse and acknowledge that the horse may have a very different feeling about what, to us, might appear an easy task. We can't use language to provide the relevant information or rationale behind any requests but we can take time for the horse to really understand what we would like and use shaping to make any task non-aversive and as easy to achieve as possible. We can reduce the use of controlling training by allowing the horse to make choices and decisions and we can pay attention to the answers we are given. We can make use of occasional free-shaping sessions to give the horse the opportunity to choose to perform behaviours to elicit rewards, without losing sight of the controlling nature of reward-based training. The goal here is for the horse to be making decisions, rather than merely performing behaviours of our choosing. And couple all of this with meeting his ethological needs as best we can. And when we do all of this, it is remarkable how much easier it is to make requests of them.
So what is the right continuum? I would put all forms of punishment, reinforcement, control and extrinsic motivation at the "bad end" and choice, empowerment, autonomy and intrinsic motivation at the "good" end. And that is the case for myself, my children and my animals. We can't avoid the "bad end" entirely but by maximising the opportunities for autonomy, we can work towards returning some of that long-lost fundamental need to our animals.
References
Virginia Axline, 1964, "Dibs: In Search of Self"
Ed Deci, 1995, "Why We Do What We Do"
Harry Harlow, 1953, "Motivation as a factor in the acquisition of new responses." In "Current theory and research on motivation (p 24-49), Lincoln, NB: University of Nebraska Press
Alfie Kohn, 1993, "Punished By Rewards"
Labels:
Behaviour
My life in horses
Beanie (Katric Katrina) a chestnut NF mare dearest Beanie Bag who was an angel for me as a child but would run my father ragged if he tried to get her in for the farrier. We were as nosey as each other and I can remember being carted in walk down a driveway as she was determined to see what was what
Skunk who started me on the journey to being a better person, who taught me so much and who I miss daily, not bad for a horse that I only intended to have for six months whilst I found a more suitable horse
Fred my beloved Fred who came on loan and stayed, who kept me going when Skunk died
Pickwick a PStG strawberry roan schoolmaster on loan darling Pickles I wish you could have stayed with us for ever but circumstances declared otherwise
Chorister the most talented horse I'll probably ever own, who healed my broken heart and taught me to love again
Each and every one of you I owe you an enormous debt, you've kept me sane, made me laugh and filled my life with joy
Skunk who started me on the journey to being a better person, who taught me so much and who I miss daily, not bad for a horse that I only intended to have for six months whilst I found a more suitable horse
Fred my beloved Fred who came on loan and stayed, who kept me going when Skunk died
Pickwick a PStG strawberry roan schoolmaster on loan darling Pickles I wish you could have stayed with us for ever but circumstances declared otherwise
Chorister the most talented horse I'll probably ever own, who healed my broken heart and taught me to love again
Each and every one of you I owe you an enormous debt, you've kept me sane, made me laugh and filled my life with joy
Labels:
Random ramblings
Second osteopath visit
Second visit, tried riding yesterday and immediately sent pony crooked so was fairly sure I was still out and I am :-( not much straighter than when I first presented but apparently less 'tight' so it is progress.
Next appt a week to ten days, then if still much the same will need a heel lift to assist me.
Will have to make a later appt as it is knocking me sideways and feels like I have cracked ribs atm
I have lordosis, a scoliosis and a wonky pelvis
Next appt a week to ten days, then if still much the same will need a heel lift to assist me.
Will have to make a later appt as it is knocking me sideways and feels like I have cracked ribs atm
I have lordosis, a scoliosis and a wonky pelvis
Labels:
Osteopath
Thursday, 13 March 2014
Wednesday, 12 March 2014
Osteopath
So this morning I went to the osteopath, a first for me was referred by my physio as pelvis was 'out' and in need of more manipulation than she could address.
Monday had to leave work early as couldn't sit or stand, spent yesterday in bed on Tramadol as nothing else was touching the pain.
So it is my psoas major which has pulled my left illeum (?) posteriorly and is the cause of my abdominal pain (nearly hit ceiling on palpation of left side) .
Very possibly this is why I had abdominal pain in 2007 and then again in 2008 which led to me being referred for ovarian scan (site of pain exactly the same)
It may also be why some 30 years ago (?) I was x-rayed following a back pain episode and told I had a scoliosis but then some years later it had 'gone' apparently it is back but he thinks it could well be the pelvis 'moving' causing the scoliosis to come and go or of course whoever told me it had gone was just wrong?
It also explains the back pain I get up between my shoulder blades
So the only question is now is this 'issue' something that has developed and with treatment/exercises will resolve or is it structural and i'll need a heel wedge to correct.
The above is my wording and may well be confused as the information was coming at me so fast, me I am just grateful to be out of severe pain.
Monday had to leave work early as couldn't sit or stand, spent yesterday in bed on Tramadol as nothing else was touching the pain.
So it is my psoas major which has pulled my left illeum (?) posteriorly and is the cause of my abdominal pain (nearly hit ceiling on palpation of left side) .
Very possibly this is why I had abdominal pain in 2007 and then again in 2008 which led to me being referred for ovarian scan (site of pain exactly the same)
It may also be why some 30 years ago (?) I was x-rayed following a back pain episode and told I had a scoliosis but then some years later it had 'gone' apparently it is back but he thinks it could well be the pelvis 'moving' causing the scoliosis to come and go or of course whoever told me it had gone was just wrong?
It also explains the back pain I get up between my shoulder blades
So the only question is now is this 'issue' something that has developed and with treatment/exercises will resolve or is it structural and i'll need a heel wedge to correct.
The above is my wording and may well be confused as the information was coming at me so fast, me I am just grateful to be out of severe pain.
Labels:
Osteopath
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)